Alexander, L. G. (1967). Practice and progress: An integrated course for pre-intermediate students.London: Longman.
Google Scholar
Andrianopolous, M. V., Darrow, K. N., & Chen, J. (2001). Multimodal standarization of voice among four multicultural populations: Formant structures. Journal of Voice, 15, 61–77.
Google Scholar
Anisfeld, M., Bogo, N., & Lambert, W. E. (1962). Evaluational reactions to accented English speech. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, 223–231.
Google Scholar
Arthur, B., Farrar, D., & Bradford, G. (1974). Evaluation reactions of college students to dialect differences in the English of Mexican-Americans. Language Speech, 17(3), 255–270.
Google Scholar
Black, J. W. (1973). The ‘phonemic’ content of backward-reproduced speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 16, 165–174.
Google Scholar
Boersma, P. (2001). Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer. Glot International, 10, 341–345.
Google Scholar
Cummings, F., & Port, R. (1998). Rhythmic constraints on stress timing in English. Journal of Phonetics, 26, 145–171.
Google Scholar
Davis, M. H., Johnsrude, I. S., Hervais-Adelman, A., Taylor, K., & McGettigan, C. (2005). Lexical information drives perceptual learning of distorted speech: Evidence from the comprehension of noise-vocoded sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 134(2), 222–241.
Google Scholar
Dellwo, V., Leemann, A., & Kolly, M.-J. (2012). Speaker idiosyncratic rhythmic features in the speech signal. Electronic Proceedings of Interspeech 2012. Portland, OR, USA, 1584–1587.
Google Scholar
Derwing, T. M., Munro, M. J., & Thomson, R. I. (2008). A longitudinal study of ESL learners’ fluency and comprehensibility development. Applied Linguistics, 29(3), 359–380.
Google Scholar
Donaldson, W. (1992). Measuring recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 121(3), 275–277.
Google Scholar
Donaldson, W. (1993). Accuracy of d’ and A’ as estimates of sensitivity. Bulletin of Psychonomic Society, 31, 271–274.
Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., & Port. R. (1981). Cross-language phonetic interference: Arabic to English. Language and Speech, 24(2), 125–146.
Google Scholar
Fourcin, A., & Dellwo, V. (2009). Rhythmic classification of languages based on voice timing. UCL Eprints. Retrieved from: http://eprints.ucl.ac.uk/15122/ accessed March 15, 2018.
Google Scholar
Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley. Kolly, M.-J., Boula de Mareüil, P., Leemann, A., & Dellwo, V. (2017). Listeners use temporal information to identify French- and English-accented speech. Speech Communication, 86, 121–134.
Google Scholar
Kolly, M.-J., & Dellwo, V. (2013). (How) can listeners identify the L1 in foreign-accented L2 speech? Travaux Neuchâtelois de Linguistique, 59, 127–148.
Google Scholar
Kolly, M.-J., & Dellwo, V. (2014). Cues to linguistic origin: The contribution of speech temporal information to foreign accent recognition. Journal of Phonetics, 42, 12–23.
Google Scholar
Laver, J. (1980). The phonetic description of voice quality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Lee, C. S., & Todd, N. P. M. (2004). Towards an auditory account of speech rhythm: Application of a model of the auditory ‘primal sketch’ to two multi-language corpora. Cognition, 93, 225–254.
Google Scholar
Lev-Ari, S., & Keysar, B. 2010. Why don’t we believe non-native speakers? The influence of accent on credibility.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 1093–1096.
Google Scholar
Lippi-Green, R. (1997). English with an accent: Language, ideology, and discrimination in the United States. London–New York: Routledge.
Google Scholar
Luke, S. G. (2017). Evaluating significance in linear mixed-effects models in R. Behavior Research Methods, 49, 1494–1502.
Google Scholar
Mennen, I. (2004). Bidirectional interference in the intonation of Dutch speakers of Greek. Journal of Phonetics, 32, 543–563.
Google Scholar
Munro, M. J., & Derwing, T. M. (2001). Modeling perceptions of the accentedness and comprehensibility of L2 speech: The role of speaking rate. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 23(4), 451–468.
Google Scholar
Munro, M. J., Derwing, T. M., & Burgess, C. S. (2010). Detection of nonnative speaker status from content-masked speech. Speech Communication, 52, 626–637.
Google Scholar
Porzuczek, A. (2012). Measuring vowel duration variability in native English speakers and Polish learners.Research in Language, 10(2), 201–214.
Google Scholar
Ramus, F., Hauser, M. D., Marc, D., Miller, C., Morris, D., & Mehler, J. (2000). Language discrimination by human newborns and by cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Science, 288, 349–351.
Google Scholar
Raupach, M. (1980). Temporal variables in first and second language speech production. In H. W. Dechert & M. Raupach (Eds.), Temporal variables in speech: Studies in honour of F. Goldman-Eisler (pp. 263–270). The Hague: Mouton Publishers.
Google Scholar
Riggenbach, H. (1991). Toward an understanding of fluency: A microanalysis of nonnative speaker conversations. Discourse Process, 14, 423–441.
Google Scholar
Ryan, E. B., & Carranza, M. A. (1975). Evaluative reactions of adolescents toward speakers of standard English and Mexican American accented English. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31(5), 855–863.
Google Scholar
Schairer, K. E. (1992). Native speaker reaction to non-native speech. Modern Language Journal, 76(3), 309–319.
Google Scholar
Searle, S. R., Casella, G., & McCulloch, C. E. (1992). Variance components. New York: Wiley.
Google Scholar
Shannon, R. V., Zeng, F.-G., Kamath, V., Wygonski, J., & Ekelid, M. (1995). Speech recognition with primarily temporal cues. Science, 270, 303–304.
Google Scholar
Stoet, G. (2010). A software package for programming psychological experiments using Linux. Behavior Research Methods, 42(4), 1096–1104.
Google Scholar
Stoet, G. (2017). A novel web-based method for running online questionnaires and reaction-time experiments. Teaching of Psychology, 44(1), 24–31.
Google Scholar
Tajima, K., Port, R., & Dalby, J. (1997). Effects of temporal correction on intelligibility of foreign accented English. Journal of Phonetics, 25, 1–24.
Google Scholar
Tilsen, S., & Arvaniti, A. (2013). Speech rhythm analysis with decomposition of the amplitude envelope: Characterizing rhythmic patterns within and across languages. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 134, 628–639.
Google Scholar
Toro, J. M., Trobalon, J. B., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2003). The use of prosodic cues in language discrimination tasks by rats. Animal Cognition, 6, 131–136.
Google Scholar
Trofimovich, P., & Baker, W. (2006). Learning second-language suprasegmentals: Effect of L2 experience on prosody and fluency characteristics of L2 speech. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 1–30.
Google Scholar
Van Lancker, D., Kreiman, J., & Emmorey, K. (1985). Familiar voice recognition: Patterns and parameters Part I: Recognition of backwards voices. Journal of Phonetics, 13, 19–38.
Google Scholar
White, L., & Mattys, S. L. (2007). Calibrating rhythm: First language and second language studies. Journal of Phonetics, 35, 501–522.
Google Scholar